Inhibitory autapse mediates anticipated synchronization between coupled neurons

Inhibitory autapse mediates anticipated synchronization between coupled neurons

Marcel A. Pinto Instituto de Física, Universidade Federal de Alagoas, Maceió, Alagoas 57072-970 Brazil    Osvaldo A. Rosso Instituto de Física, Universidade Federal de Alagoas, Maceió, Alagoas 57072-970 Brazil Departamento de Informática en Salud, Hospital Italiano de Buenos Aires CONICET, C1199ABB, Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina. Complex Systems Group, Facultad de Ingeniería y Ciencias Aplicadas, Universidad de los Andes, Avenida Monseñor Álvaro del Portillo 12.455, Las Condes, Santiago, Chile.    Fernanda S. Matias Instituto de Física, Universidade Federal de Alagoas, Maceió, Alagoas 57072-970 Brazil
Abstract

Two identical autonomous dynamical systems unidirectionally coupled in a sender-receiver configuration can exhibit anticipated synchronization (AS) if the Receiver neuron (R) also receives a delayed negative self-feedback. Recently, AS was shown to occur in a three-neuron motif with standard chemical synapses where the delayed inhibition was provided by an interneuron. Here we show that a two-neuron model in the presence of an inhibitory autapse, which is a massive self-innervation present in the cortical architecture, may present AS. The GABAergic autapse regulates the internal dynamics of the Receiver neuron and acts as the negative delayed self-feedback required by dynamical systems in order to exhibit AS. In this biologically plausible scenario, a smooth transition from the usual delayed synchronization (DS) to AS typically occurs when the inhibitory conductance is increased. The phenomenon is shown to be robust when model parameters are varied within a physiological range. For extremely large values of the inhibitory autapse the system undergoes to a phase-drift regime in which the Receiver is faster than the Sender. Furthermore, we show that the inhibitory autapse promotes a faster internal dynamics of the free-running Receiver when the two neurons are uncoupled, which could be the mechanism underlying anticipated synchronization and the DS-AS transition.

pacs:
87.18.Sn, 87.19.ll, 87.19.lm
thanks: fernanda@fis.ufal.br

I Introduction

Flexible communication among neuronal networks requires that the same anatomical connectivity can present different functional connectivity patterns Battaglia et al. (1998). At the neuronal level, microcircuits producing coherent oscillations can be viewed as building blocks of the effective network dynamics in the brain. This means that the structural connectivity of neuronal motifs and the intrinsic excitability of each neuron modulate the information flow in the network Pariz et al. (2018). In particular, the presence of autaptic connections (or autapses), which could be synapses from the axon of a neuron to its own somato-dendritic domain, have been shown to influence both the firing rhythm of the neuron and the synchronization properties of the network Pawelzik et al. (2003); Tamas et al. (1997); Wang et al. (2014); Herrmann and Klaus (2004).

The first observation of autapses was reported more than forty five years ago in pyramidal neurons from the neocortex Van Der Loos and Glaser (1972). Since then, neurobiologists have changed the status of these connections from not relevant structures that only appears in experimental cultures to a massive self-innervation present in the cortical architecture Herrmann and Klaus (2004). Inhibitory autapses can be axo-axonic, axo-dendritic or dendo-dendritic and have been found in the cerebellum  Pouzat and Marty (1998, 1999), neocortex Tamas et al. (1997) and hippocampal cultures Bekkers and Stevens (1991); Bekkers (2002). However, autaptic connections are rarely included in cortical circuit diagrams and it is not known whether they present any kind of plasticity Deleuze et al. (2014). Even though the functional role of the autapses is still lacking, it has been speculated for decades that they could act as self-inhibitory function Bekkers and Stevens (1991).

Here we propose that autapses could act as the negative delayed self-feedback required by unidirectionally coupled dynamical systems in order to present anticipated synchronization (AS) Voss (2000). AS has been shown to be a stable but counter-intuitive solution of a system described by the following set of equations:

(1)

is a vector function that describes the autonomous dynamical system, is the coupling matrix and the delayed term is the self-feedback Voss (2000). The solution , characterizes the anticipated synchronization and has been verified in a variety of theoretical Voss (2000, 2001a, 2001b); Kostur et al. (2005); Masoller and Zanette (2001); Hernández-García et al. (2002); Hayashi et al. (2016) and experimental Sivaprakasam et al. (2001); Ciszak et al. (2009); Tang and Liu (2003) studies. The striking aspect of this solution is that in a time the Receiver predicts the state of the Sender in a future time .

The first verification of anticipation in a neuronal model was done by Ciszak et al. Ciszak et al. (2003) with two FitzHugh-Nagumo neurons diffusively coupled in such a way that, apart from an external stimulus, they could be modeled by Eq. 1. AS has also been verified between two Rulkov map-based neurons in the presence of synaptic delay and a memory term as the self-feedback  Sausedo-Solorio and Pisarchik (2014), as well as in a three-neuron motif coupled by chemical synapses in which the self-feedback was mediated by an interneuron Matias et al. (2011). The later result has been extended to show that AS is robust against noise Matias et al. (2016) and spike-timing dependent plasticity Matias et al. (2015). AS has also been verified in cortical-like populations in the presence of excitatory and inhibitory neurons Matias et al. (2014), which could explain a positive and unidirectional Granger causality and a negative phase difference between cortical areas of a non-human primate Matias et al. (2014); Montani et al. (2015); Brovelli et al. (2004); Salazar et al. (2012).

Furthermore, it has been shown that the internal dynamics of the Receiver neuron may control the relative phase between the Sender and the Receiver neurons if they are synchronized Hayashi et al. (2016). In fact, anticipation in spike synchronization has been shown between two unidirectionally coupled nonidentical chaotic neurons when the mean frequency of the free post-synaptic neuron is greater than the pre-synaptic one Pyragienè and Pyragas (2013). AS has also been verified between two Hodgkin-Huxley neurons coupled by a nonlinear excitatory synapse in which the depolarization levels (but not the synaptic conductance) determine the phase differences Simonov et al. (2014). More recently, AS has also been verified in population neural models representing a microcircuit of sthe ongbird brain involved in song production in which the Receiver dynamics is faster than the Senders Dima et al. (2018).

Here we show that a two-neuron motif with an inhibitory autapse in the post-synaptic neuron can present an anticipatory regime. The autapse can act as a dynamical self-feedback and it can regulate the internal dynamics of the Receiver neuron. We employ numerical simulations to show that GABAergic autapses can be a biologically plausible mechanism for AS in neuronal motifs. For very small values of the inhibitory conductance the neurons synchronize with the usual pre-post order which is called the delayed synchronization regime (DS). As we increase the inhibition the system presents a conductance-induced DS-AS transition. In Sec. II we describe our simple motif as well as the neuronal and synaptic models. In Sec. III, we report our results, showing that AS can be mediated by an inhibitory autapse in physiological regions of parameter space. Concluding remarks and briefly discussion of the significance of our findings for neuroscience are presented in Sec. IV.

Ii Model

Figure 1: (Color online) Neuronal motif: Sender (S) and Receiver (R) neurons unidirectionally connected by an excitatory chemical synapse. The Receiver neuron presents an inhibitory chemical autapse which is a dynamical version of the negative delayed self-feedback in Eq. 1 .

Our motif is composed of two Izhikevich neuron models Izhikevich (2003) and chemical synapses as shown in Fig.1. The spiking activity of the Sender (S) neuron is described by the equations bellow:

(2)
(3)

The Receiver (R) neuron has similar equations but with two extra terms which accounts for the excitatory (E) and self-inhibitory (I) synaptic currents:

(4)
(5)

For both neurons the reset condition is given by: if  mV, then and . The variable is the equivalent of the membrane potential of the neuron and represents a membrane recovery variable. The subscript indexes each neuron . We employ the same set of parameters for both neurons: , , and . is the external constant current which determines the neuronal firing rate.

The R neuron is subject to one excitatory synapses from the S neuron mediated by AMPA receptors with synaptic conductance . R is also subject to the inhibitory autapse mediated by GABA receptors with autaptic conductance . The AMPA and GABA reversal potentials are respectively  mV and  mV. The fraction of bound (i.e. open) synaptic receptors is modeled by a first-order kinetic dynamics:

(6)

where and are rate constants. The index represents the excitatory synapse and the inhibitory autapse . is the neurotransmitter concentration in the synaptic cleft. In its simplest model it is an instantaneous function of the pre-synaptic potential  Koch and Segev (1998):

(7)

In our model  mM,  mV,  mV.

Unless otherwise stated, the rate constants are  mMms,  ms ,  mMms, and  ms similarly to the ones in Refs. Koch and Segev (1998); Matias et al. (2011). However, these values depend on a number of different factors and can vary significantly Geiger et al. (1997); Kraushaar and Jonas (2000).

(a) (b)
Figure 2: (Color online) Characterizing the phase-locking and phase-drift regimes. (a) Time series of the Sender and the Receiver neuron after a transient time for different values of the autaptic inhibition and fixed external current pA. (b) The spike-timing difference between S and R as a function of the i-th cycle. A pre-post firing order characterizes the usual delayed (DS) synchronization regime (upper panels,  nS). The anticipated synchronization regime (AS) occurs when the Receiver neuron fires before the Sender (middle panels,  nS). When neurons fire with different frequencies, the spike timing difference changes every cycle and the neurons are in a phase-drift (PD) regime (bottom panels,  nS).

Iii Results

Initially, we describe our results for the scenario where both neurons are subjected to a constant current  pA. Unless otherwise stated we keep the excitatory conductance fixed as  nS which is larger enough to promote a phase-locking regime between the two neurons when there is no autapse. For different sets of inhibitory conductance values our motif can exhibit different behaviors as shown in Fig. 2. In order to characterize each regime, we define as the time in which the membrane potential of the Sender neuron is larger than the threshold ( mV) in the -th cycle (i.e. its -th spike time), and as the spike time of the Receiver neuron which is nearest to . The spike-timing difference between S and R is defined as:

(8)

When converges to a constant value , a phase-locked regime is reached Strogatz (1997). By definition, if we say that the system exhibits delayed synchronization (DS) and the neurons fire in the usual pre-post order (see upper panels in Fig. 2a and b, for  nS) . If we say that anticipated synchronization (AS) occurs and the neurons fire in a non-intuitive post-pre order (see middle panels in Fig. 2a and b, for  nS). If does not converge to a fixed value, the system is in a phase-drift (PD) regime Strogatz (1997) (see bottom panels Fig. 2a and b, for  nS).

Figure 3: (Color online) Transition from delayed to anticipated synchronization regime. Spiking time difference normalized by the period of the phase-locking as a function of inhibitory conductance for different values of external current . Positive values of characterizes DS, whereas represents the AS regime. The stars mark the end of the phase-locking regime and the beginning of the phase-drift (PD). In PD the value of in each cycle does not converge to a fixed value as shown in Fig. 2(b).

iii.1 The conductance induced transition from DS to AS

The spike-timing difference is a continuous and smooth function of . The transition from delayed to anticipated regime through a zero-lag regime may be induced by the inhibitory autaptic conductance as shown in Fig. 3. To compare the transition for different values of external current we normalized by the firing period which depends on . Both AS and the DS-AS transition can be found in a large region of parameter space. Typically, AS occurs for . The relation between , excitatory and inhibitory conductances for different values of is shown in Fig. 4. Results are independent of initial conditions and perturbations. The phase-locking regimes are always reached after a transient time.

It is worth to mention that the presence of the autapse in the post-synaptic neuron, even for very small values of inhibitory conductance, decreases the spike-timing difference between the neurons. This means that autapses could be biologically important to overcome synaptic delays between distant areas. Moreover, for enough inhibition, the neurons can fire exactly at the same time. Therefore, autapse can be also considered as a mechanism to promote zero-lag synchronizationGray et al. (1989); Gollo et al. (2014) which does not require bidirectional connections.

We show here that autaptic weights may control the spike-timing difference between pre and post-synaptic neurons through a conductance induced DS-AS transition. This result can be computationally considered as the other way around of the spike-timing dependent plasticity (STDP) Abbott and Nelson (2000), which is a biological mechanism that allows the spike-timing differences between pre and post-synaptic neurons to control changes in the synaptic weights.

Figure 4: (Color online) Normalized spike-timing difference (right bar) in the (,) projection of parameter space for different values of external current : DS (blue, mostly at the upper part in which and ), AS (red, middle), and PD (white, meaning that no stationary value of was found).

iii.2 GABAergic autapse promotes a faster internal dynamics

Occasionally inhibitory synapses are viewed as a biological mechanism to avoid neurons to fire. However, we show here that the GABAergic autapse can allow the neuron to fire with higher frequencies. In the phase-locking regimes (both DS and AS) the period of the Receiver is the same of the Sender ( which is the period of a neuron without an autapse ). As we increase the inhibition, the spike-timing difference decreases and eventually, the system loses the phase-locking regime. For nS and  pA, as we increase the system exhibits a second transition from AS to a phase-drift regime in which (see Fig. 5(a)). This means that in the PD regime the Receiver fires faster than the Sender. The return map of the Receiver period in each cycle versus characterizes the phase-drift (see Fig. 5(b)) which is a quasi-periodic regime. This counter-intuitive effect of inhibition has also been reported for a neuron participating in a inhibitory loop mediated by an interneuron Matias et al. (2011).

For small values of external current  pA and  nS the membrane potential of the Receiver neuron does not reach the condition for reset, which means that it does not fire a spike. Thus, it is not possible to define . In fact, for this region of the parameter space the inhibition can silence the neuron.

The effect of the autapse in the internal dynamics of the Receiver can also be studied for the uncoupled case . For this situation, as we increase , the firing period of the neuron subjected to the autapse initially decreases and for any value of external current (see Fig. 6). This result is in agreement with previous studies showing that the mechanism underlying anticipated synchronization is a faster internal dynamics of the Receiver Hayashi et al. (2016); Pyragienè and Pyragas (2013); Porta et al. (2019).

The inhibitory autapse can also promote the transition from DS to AS when the sender and the receiver neurons are not described by the same equations or parameters. If the receiver neuron is a fast spiking neuron Izhikevich (2003) (with model parameters , , and ) the transition from positive to negative is still continuous and smooth. In such case the external current in each neuron should be different in order to ensure that their natural frequencies are similar. For example, if  pA,  pA,  nS,  ms,  ms the system exhibits DS if and AS if  nS (data not shown).

Moreover, if the receiver neuron is modeled as a two-compartment neuron, the system can also exhibit AS. We have considered that the compartments are connected by an electrical synpase with synaptic current given by and the second compartment sends an inhibitory chemical synapse to the first. For  nS,  nS,  pA the system undergoes a transition from DS to AS when we increase from zero to  nS (data not shown). Further investigation on how anticipated synchronization depends on more complex dendritic trees, other types of neurons and slower synaptic currents mediated by NMDA and GABA are beyond the scope of this paper and should be reported elsewhere in the future.

(a) (b)
Figure 5: (Color online) Characterizing the phase-drift regime induced by the inhibitory autapse: the Receiver is faster than the Sender. (a) The mean period of the Receiver normalized by its own period when , coincides with the mean period of the Sender for DS and AS regimes, but it is smaller for PD. (b) In PD, the return map of the period in each cycle of the Receiver is consistent with a quasi-periodic system.
Figure 6: (Color online) Inhibitory autapse promotes a faster internal dynamical of the free-running Receiver, which could be the mechanism promoting AS and the transition from DS to AS. For the uncoupled situation () the period of the free-running Receiver (normalized by its own period for , ) decreases as we increase the inhibitory conductance . For external currents  pA the minimum value of can also be related to the minimum value of in Fig. 3. For  pA the system undergoes a transition from AS to PD regime before reaches the minima.

Iv Concluding remarks

To summarize, we have shown that the presence of a GABAergic autapse can promote anticipated synchronization between two coupled neurons. Here, the inhibitory self-innervation acts as a dynamical delayed negative self-feedback Voss (2000); Ciszak et al. (2003) or the inhibitory loop mediated by a third neuron Matias et al. (2011, 2014) required by previously studied systems in order to exhibit AS. This also means that even in a very simple microcircuit of two neurons, the characteristic time scale and conductance of the excitatory connection are not enough to determine the phase difference between two synchronized neurons. Moreover, autapses could be a biologically plausible mechanism to overcome synaptic delays.

We have also shown that the neuron subjected to the inhibitory autapse can fire faster than without it. Such a result is in consonance with previous studies Hayashi et al. (2016); Pyragienè and Pyragas (2013); Porta et al. (2019) showing that the mechanism underlying AS and the DS-AS transition can be a faster internal dynamics of the Receiver system. Moreover, previous work on AS have investigate the phenomenon for neurons with firing rates larger than  Hz Matias et al. (2011, 2015). Typically cortical neurons in vivo do not fire at rates greater than  Hz. Here we show that AS can occur for firing rates around  Hz which means that AS is a robust phenomenon at different time scales.

The DS-AS transition could possibly explain commonly reported short latency in visual systems Orban et al. (1985); Nowak et al. (1995); Kerzel and Gegenfurtner (2003); Jancke et al. (2004); Puccini et al. (2007); Martinez et al. (2014), olfactory circuits Rospars et al. (2014), songbirds brain Dima et al. (2018) and human perception Stepp and Turvey (2010, 2017). Differently from the first papers about AS Voss (2000); Ciszak et al. (2003), here the anticipation time is not hard-wired in the dynamical equations, but rather emerges from the autapse dynamics. The spike-timing difference between post- and pre-synaptic neurons decrease as we increase the inhibitory autaptic conductance. Eventually the system presents a conductance induced transition from delayed to anticipated regimes similarly to what has been reported in a three neuron motif Matias et al. (2011, 2014). This AS-DS transition could synergistically work together with spike-timing-dependent plasticity Abbott and Nelson (2000); Clopath et al. (2010); Matias et al. (2015) to determine the circuit dynamics and synaptic conductances. Including effects from synaptic plasticity in the model presented here is a natural next step which we are currently pursuing.

Acknowledgements.
The authors thank FAPEAL and UFAL (for PIBIC grant), CNPq (grant 432429/2016-6) and CAPES (grant 88881.120309/2016-01) for finnancial support.

References

  • Battaglia et al. (1998) D. Battaglia, A. Witt, F. Wolf, and T. Geisel, PLoS Comput. Biol. 393, 18 (1998).
  • Pariz et al. (2018) A. Pariz, Z. G. Esfahani, S. S. Parsi, A. Valizadeh, S. Canals, and C. R. Mirasso, NeuroImage 166, 349 (2018).
  • Pawelzik et al. (2003) H. Pawelzik, D. Hughes, and A. Thomson, The Journal of physiology 546, 701 (2003).
  • Tamas et al. (1997) G. Tamas, E. H. Buhl, and P. Somogyi, Journal of Neuroscience 17, 6352 (1997).
  • Wang et al. (2014) H. Wang, L. Wang, Y. Chen, and Y. Chen, Chaos: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Nonlinear Science 24, 033122 (2014).
  • Herrmann and Klaus (2004) C. S. Herrmann and A. Klaus, International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos 14, 623 (2004).
  • Van Der Loos and Glaser (1972) H. Van Der Loos and E. M. Glaser, Brain research 48, 355 (1972).
  • Pouzat and Marty (1998) C. Pouzat and A. Marty, The Journal of Physiology 509, 777 (1998).
  • Pouzat and Marty (1999) C. Pouzat and A. Marty, Journal of Neuroscience 19, 1675 (1999).
  • Bekkers and Stevens (1991) J. Bekkers and C. Stevens, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 88, 7834 (1991).
  • Bekkers (2002) J. M. Bekkers, Current biology 12, R648 (2002).
  • Deleuze et al. (2014) C. Deleuze, A. Pazienti, and A. Bacci, Current opinion in neurobiology 26, 64 (2014).
  • Voss (2000) H. U. Voss, Phys. Rev. E 61, 5115 (2000).
  • Voss (2001a) H. U. Voss, Phys. Rev. E 64, 039904 (2001a).
  • Voss (2001b) H. U. Voss, Phys. Rev. Lett. 87, 014102 (2001b).
  • Kostur et al. (2005) M. Kostur, P. Hänggi, P. Talkner, and J. L. Mateos, Phys. Rev. E 72, 036210 (2005).
  • Masoller and Zanette (2001) C. Masoller and D. H. Zanette, Physica A 300, 359 (2001).
  • Hernández-García et al. (2002) E. Hernández-García, C. Masoller, and C. Mirasso, Phys. Lett. A 295, 39 (2002).
  • Hayashi et al. (2016) Y. Hayashi, S. J. Nasuto, and H. Eberle, Physical Review E 93, 052229 (2016).
  • Sivaprakasam et al. (2001) S. Sivaprakasam, E. M. Shahverdiev, P. S. Spencer, and K. A. Shore, Phys. Rev. Lett. 87, 154101 (2001).
  • Ciszak et al. (2009) M. Ciszak, C. R. Mirasso, R. Toral, and O. Calvo, Phys. Rev. E 79, 046203 (2009).
  • Tang and Liu (2003) S. Tang and J. M. Liu, Phys. Rev. Lett. 90, 194101 (2003).
  • Ciszak et al. (2003) M. Ciszak, O. Calvo, C. Masoller, C. R. Mirasso, and R. Toral, Phys. Rev. Lett. 90, 204102 (2003).
  • Sausedo-Solorio and Pisarchik (2014) J. Sausedo-Solorio and A. Pisarchik, Physics Letters A 378, 2108 (2014).
  • Matias et al. (2011) F. S. Matias, P. V. Carelli, C. R. Mirasso, and M. Copelli, Phys. Rev. E 84, 021922 (2011).
  • Matias et al. (2016) F. S. Matias, L. L. Gollo, P. V. Carelli, C. R. Mirasso, and M. Copelli, Phys. Rev. E 94, 042411 (2016).
  • Matias et al. (2015) F. S. Matias, P. V. Carelli, C. R. Mirasso, and M. Copelli, PloS one 10, e0140504 (2015).
  • Matias et al. (2014) F. S. Matias, L. L. Gollo, P. V. Carelli, S. L. Bressler, M. Copelli, and C. R. Mirasso, NeuroImage 99, 411 (2014).
  • Montani et al. (2015) F. Montani, O. A. Rosso, F. S. Matias, S. L. Bressler, and C. R. Mirasso, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. A 373, 20150110 (2015).
  • Brovelli et al. (2004) A. Brovelli, M. Ding, A. Ledberg, Y. Chen, R. Nakamura, and S. L. Bressler, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101, 9849 (2004).
  • Salazar et al. (2012) R. F. Salazar, N. M. Dotson, S. L. Bressler, and C. M. Gray, Science 338, 1097 (2012).
  • Pyragienè and Pyragas (2013) T. Pyragienè and K. Pyragas, Nonlinear Dynamics 74, 297 (2013).
  • Simonov et al. (2014) A. Y. Simonov, S. Y. Gordleeva, A. Pisarchik, and V. Kazantsev, JETP Letters 98, 632 (2014).
  • Dima et al. (2018) G. C. Dima, M. Copelli, and G. B. Mindlin, International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos 28, 1830025 (2018).
  • Izhikevich (2003) E. Izhikevich, IEEE Transaction on Neural Networks 14, 1569 (2003).
  • Koch and Segev (1998) C. Koch and I. Segev, eds., Methods in Neuronal Modeling: From Ions to Networks (MIT Press, 1998), 2nd ed.
  • Geiger et al. (1997) J. R. P. Geiger, J. Lübke, A. Roth, M. Frotscher, and P. Jonas, Neuron 18, 1009 (1997).
  • Kraushaar and Jonas (2000) U. Kraushaar and P. Jonas, The Journal of Neuroscience 20, 5594 (2000).
  • Strogatz (1997) S. H. Strogatz, Nonlinear Dynamics and Chaos: with Applications to Physics, Biology, Chemistry and Engineering (Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA, 1997).
  • Gray et al. (1989) C. M. Gray, P. König, A. K. Engel, W. Singer, et al., Nature 338, 334 (1989).
  • Gollo et al. (2014) L. L. Gollo, C. Mirasso, O. Sporns, and M. Breakspear, PLoS Computational Biology 10, e1003548 (2014).
  • Abbott and Nelson (2000) L. F. Abbott and S. B. Nelson, Nature Neuroscience 3, 1178 (2000).
  • Porta et al. (2019) L. D. Porta, F. S. Matias, A. J. dos Santos, A. Alonso, P. V. Carelli, M. Copelli, and C. R. Mirasso (2019), submitted.
  • Orban et al. (1985) G. A. Orban, K. Hoffmann, and J. Duysens, Journal of Neurophysiology 54, 1026 (1985).
  • Nowak et al. (1995) L. Nowak, M. Munk, P. Girard, and J. Bullier, Visual neuroscience 12, 371 (1995).
  • Kerzel and Gegenfurtner (2003) D. Kerzel and K. R. Gegenfurtner, Current Biology 13, 1975 (2003).
  • Jancke et al. (2004) D. Jancke, W. Erlhagen, G. Schöner, and H. R. Dinse, The Journal of physiology 556, 971 (2004).
  • Puccini et al. (2007) G. D. Puccini, M. V. Sanchez-Vives, and A. Compte, PLoS computational biology 3, e82 (2007).
  • Martinez et al. (2014) L. M. Martinez, M. Molano-Mazo, X. Wang, F. T. Sommer, and J. Hirsch, Neuron 81, 943 (2014).
  • Rospars et al. (2014) J.-P. Rospars, A. Grémiaux, D. Jarriault, A. Chaffiol, C. Monsempes, N. Deisig, S. Anton, P. Lucas, and D. Martinez, PLoS Computational Biology 10, e1003975 (2014).
  • Stepp and Turvey (2010) N. Stepp and M. T. Turvey, Cognitive Systems Research 11, 148 (2010).
  • Stepp and Turvey (2017) N. Stepp and M. T. Turvey, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 43, 914 (2017).
  • Clopath et al. (2010) C. Clopath, L. Büsing, E. Vasilaki, and W. Gerstner, Nature Neuroscience 13, 344 (2010).
Comments 0
Request Comment
You are adding the first comment!
How to quickly get a good reply:
  • Give credit where it’s due by listing out the positive aspects of a paper before getting into which changes should be made.
  • Be specific in your critique, and provide supporting evidence with appropriate references to substantiate general statements.
  • Your comment should inspire ideas to flow and help the author improves the paper.

The better we are at sharing our knowledge with each other, the faster we move forward.
""
The feedback must be of minimum 40 characters and the title a minimum of 5 characters
   
Add comment
Cancel
Loading ...
379672
This is a comment super asjknd jkasnjk adsnkj
Upvote
Downvote
""
The feedback must be of minumum 40 characters
The feedback must be of minumum 40 characters
Submit
Cancel

You are asking your first question!
How to quickly get a good answer:
  • Keep your question short and to the point
  • Check for grammar or spelling errors.
  • Phrase it like a question
Test
Test description